Skip to content

What is an example of disuse theory? The Case of Forgetting Memory

3 min read

According to research from the University of Waterloo, a new foreign language can become increasingly inaccessible in a person's memory the less they use it. This phenomenon perfectly illustrates what is an example of disuse theory—the idea that memories or knowledge not used or retrieved over a period of time become increasingly difficult to access, eventually leading to forgetting. This principle is commonly known as the "use it or lose it" concept in cognitive psychology.

Quick Summary

Disuse theory is the psychological concept that memories fade over time if they are not actively retrieved. A common illustration is forgetting a language or academic material after long periods without practice. The idea differentiates between a memory's long-term strength and its current accessibility, which decreases over time.

Key Points

  • Forgetting a Language: A powerful example of disuse theory is forgetting a foreign language after a long period of not speaking it, as the neural pathways for retrieval weaken.

  • Decay of Memory Trace: Disuse theory suggests that the physiological "memory trace" (or engram) fades over time without repeated activation.

  • Storage vs. Retrieval Strength: The newer disuse theory distinguishes between a memory's permanent storage strength and its temporary retrieval strength, which decreases with disuse.

  • Practice for Memory Maintenance: The "use it or lose it" principle is a core takeaway, showing that active retrieval is necessary to maintain memory accessibility.

  • Interference vs. Disuse: Disuse theory is distinct from interference theory, which attributes forgetting to competing memories, although both can contribute to poor recall.

  • Relevance to Learning: Understanding disuse theory informs effective learning strategies, emphasizing active recall and spaced repetition to combat forgetting.

In This Article

Understanding the Disuse Theory

The disuse theory, also known as memory decay theory, posits that the physiological or neural traces of memories fade with the passage of time if they are not reactivated through retrieval. While time is the primary factor, the underlying process is a gradual weakening of the neural connections that form the memory trace, also known as the engram. This theory provides an intuitive explanation for why people forget things, from basic facts learned in school to the details of past experiences. The core idea is that the memory itself doesn't disappear entirely but becomes less accessible, similar to how a rarely used footpath becomes overgrown with weeds.

For example, consider an individual who studied French in high school but has not spoken it since graduating. After 15 years of disuse, their ability to recall vocabulary, conjugate verbs, and hold a conversation would have significantly deteriorated. This is a powerful, real-world illustration of disuse theory in action. The individual's long-term memory may still contain fragments of their French knowledge, but the neural pathways to retrieve that information have weakened, making access much harder.

The "New Theory of Disuse" in Cognitive Science

In the 1990s, psychologists Robert and Elizabeth Bjork introduced a refinement to the classic disuse theory, known as the "new theory of disuse". Their model differentiates between two types of memory strength: storage strength and retrieval strength.

  • Storage strength: Represents how well-learned and deeply embedded a memory is in the brain. This value generally increases with learning and is thought to be largely permanent.
  • Retrieval strength: Represents how easily a memory can be accessed or recalled at a given moment. This strength is highly susceptible to decay over time and can fluctuate based on recent retrieval attempts.

This distinction helps explain a critical aspect of forgetting: the difference between a memory's long-term durability and its current accessibility. The high-school French student likely still has high storage strength for their language skills, but low retrieval strength due to lack of practice.

Disuse vs. Interference Theory

While disuse theory focuses on the passage of time, interference theory offers an alternative explanation for forgetting, suggesting that memories are not lost but are rather blocked by other competing memories. Often, both mechanisms are at play.

Feature Disuse (Decay) Theory Interference Theory
Mechanism Memories fade or the neural trace weakens due to the passage of time. Forgetting is caused by other learned information blocking access to the target memory.
Core Analogy An overgrown path from lack of use. Trying to find an old book on a shelf cluttered with new books.
Primary Cause Time and lack of retrieval. Conflicting information (either before or after learning).
Examples Forgetting the name of an obscure historical figure you haven't thought about in years. Mixing up your old phone number with your new one (proactive interference).

Conclusion: The "Use It or Lose It" Principle in Practice

Whether learning a new skill, studying for an exam, or trying to retain a new employee's name, the disuse theory has practical applications. The key takeaway is the "use it or lose it" principle. By actively retrieving information—not just reviewing it passively—you can maintain and even strengthen its retrieval pathways. This is the basis for effective study techniques like spaced repetition and active recall. Understanding disuse theory provides crucial insight into the mechanics of memory and forgetting, guiding us toward more effective learning strategies in all aspects of life. To dive deeper into the implications for education, the Bjork Learning and Forgetting Lab has extensively researched these topics.

Frequently Asked Questions

The theory of disuse, or decay theory, is the psychological concept that the memory traces for learned information fade over time if they are not regularly retrieved or accessed.

A common example is a piano player who hasn't practiced for several years. While they remember the general concepts of playing, their fingers and muscle memory are no longer as agile, and they have forgotten how to play many complex pieces, illustrating how a skill can decay with disuse.

The theory explains forgetting by suggesting that the neurological connections (memory traces) that form a memory naturally degrade over time when they are not reinforced through practice or recall. The memory doesn't disappear instantly but becomes progressively harder to access.

While the basic principle of memory decay is recognized, the original disuse theory is considered too simplistic on its own. Modern cognitive science, such as the New Theory of Disuse, incorporates more nuanced concepts like retrieval and storage strength to provide a more complete explanation of forgetting.

The best way to prevent memory decay is to actively use or retrieve the information you want to retain. This can be done through techniques like active recall (testing yourself), spaced repetition (reviewing material at increasing intervals), and applying the knowledge in new contexts.

Disuse theory suggests memories fade due to the passive passage of time, while interference theory posits that forgetting occurs because other memories block or interfere with the retrieval of the target memory. Both can contribute to forgetting, but they represent different mechanisms.

Yes, a memory with low retrieval strength can often be recovered. Just because a memory is not easily accessible does not mean it is completely gone. Using a strong retrieval cue can sometimes reactivate a dormant memory and restore its accessibility.

References

  1. 1
  2. 2
  3. 3
  4. 4

Medical Disclaimer

This content is for informational purposes only and should not replace professional medical advice. Always consult a qualified healthcare provider regarding personal health decisions.